YouTube // McGill University

Urban Birds Are Sharper Than Country Birds, Study Suggests

YouTube // McGill University

As Newsies and Pizza Rat can tell you: you’ve got to be clever and tough if you want to make it in the city. Sure, it’s common knowledge, but it’s also scientifically true, at least for birds. Ornithologists say that urban finches are both smarter and more disease-resistant than their country cousins. The researchers recently published their report on the subject in the journal Behavioral Ecology.

Jean-Nicolas Audet and his colleagues wanted to compare the birds' problem-solving skills and their immunities. To do so, they needed to find members of the same species living in two different environments. With populations of bullfinches present in both developed and open areas, as well as an ornithological research station, Barbados made a perfect test site for them.

The researchers used gentle-capture nets called mist nets to capture 53 Barbados bullfinches (Loxigilla barbadensis) from eight areas on the island—four rural and four urban. They brought the birds back to the lab and gave them a few days to settle in. Once the birds were acclimated, the tests commenced.

Audet and his colleagues rigged up a number of bird problems, each with a seed goal. In one puzzle, a bird could only reach a cup of seeds by using a stick to pull the cup closer. In another, the bird had to remove a lid or pull a hook to open a little drawer full of seeds. Each bird went up to 15 rounds with each test, with a researcher recording their successes and failures.

To test the finches’ disease resistance, the researchers injected each bird with a tiny dose of an immune-activating protein called phytohemagglutinin (PHA). Animals with strong immune systems who are given PHA will have a little inflammation at the injection site, so the ornithologists measured each bird’s wing to check for swelling.

When all the tests had been scored and the results analyzed, the city birds emerged as clear winners in both cleverness and immunity—a fact that surprised the researchers.

"Since urban birds were better at problem-solving,” Audet said in a press statement, “we expected that there would be a trade-off and that the immunity would be lower, just because we assumed that you can't be good at everything. It seems that in this case, the urban birds have it all."

nextArticle.image_alt|e
iStock
Crafty Crows Can Build Tools From Memory
iStock
iStock

Scientists have discovered yet another reason to never get on a crow's bad side. According to new research reported by Gizmodo, members of at least one crow species can build tools from memory, rather than just copying the behavior of other crows—adding to the long list of impressive skills that set these corvids apart.

For the new study, published in the journal Scientific Reports, an international team of scientists looked at New Caledonian crows, a species known for its tool usage. New Caledonian crows use sticks to pick grubs out of logs, sometimes stashing these twigs away for later. Tools are so important to their lifestyle that their beaks even evolved to hold them. But how exactly the crows know to use tools—that is, whether the behavior is just an imitation or knowledge passed down through generations—has remained unclear until now.

The researchers set up the experiment by teaching eight crows to drop pieces of paper into a box in exchange for food. The birds eventually learned that they would only be rewarded if they dispensed either large sheets of paper measuring 40-millimeters-by-60 millimeters or smaller sheets that were 15-millimeters-by-25 millimeters. After the crows had adapted and started using sheets of either size, all the paper was taken away from them and replaced with one sheet that was too big for the box.

The crows knew exactly what to do: They ripped up the sheet until it matched one of the two sizes they had used to earn their food before and inserted it into the dispenser. They were able to do this with out looking at the sheets they had used previously, which suggests they had access to a visual memory of the tools. This supports the "mental template matching" theory—a belief among some crow experts that New Caledonian crows can form a mental image of a tool just by watching another crow use it and later recreate the tool on their own, thus passing along the template to other birds including their own offspring.

This is the first time mental template matching has been observed in birds, but anyone familiar with crow intelligence shouldn't be surprised: They've also been known to read traffic lights, recognize faces, nurse grudges, and hold funerals for their dead.

[h/t Gizmodo]

nextArticle.image_alt|e
Jana Mueller
Ravens Can Figure Out When Someone Is Spying on Them
Jana Mueller
Jana Mueller

Corvids, the family of birds that includes crows and ravens, are canny beasts. They've been known to exercise self-control, count, hold grudges, and more. Now, new research suggests they possess at least a rudimentary Theory of Mind—the ability to attribute mental states to others.

A study in Nature finds that ravens can tell when someone else can see them, guarding their food when a peephole to their cache is open. While previous research suggested that birds might have an awareness of other animals' mental states, the results have been inconclusive. The Nature study is evidence that corvids can do more than just track other birds' gaze; they may understand the concept of "seeing."

Vienna-based researchers set up two rooms separated by windows that could be closed with covers. These covers had peepholes in them that could also be opened or closed. First, the 10 ravens were each allowed to cache food, while other birds were in the next room and the windows were open or closed. Then, they were trained to look through the peepholes to find food in the other room, so that they knew that the holes could be used to see through the window covers. Afterwards, each of the ravens was again presented with food with one of the two peepholes open. The adjacent observation room didn't have any birds in it, but the researchers played the sounds of another raven recorded during one of the previous trials.

When the birds heard the sounds of another raven in the next room, and the peephole was open, the birds behaved as if they knew they were being watched—they hid their cache of food quickly and didn't add more food to it as often, as if they knew that it might be compromised. However, they behaved normally when the peephole was closed.

This suggests that ravens don't just track their competitors' gaze to know when they’re being watched, but can infer from past experience when they can be seen.

SECTIONS

arrow
LIVE SMARTER
More from mental floss studios